Gay
Change
Sexual orientation from Wikipedia, the
free encyclopedia
1 Complexities
and
terminology
2 History
of sexuality
3 Orientation
concepts
4 Psychological
and sociological
viewpoints
5 Biological
viewpoint
6 Religious
and moral
viewpoints
7 Sexual
orientation as a
"construction"
8 Sexual
identity
(self-identification)
9 See
also
10 References
11 External
links
How many gay people are
there?
Attempts To Change Sexual
Orientation "Reparative therapy."
Bibliography for Facts About Sexual
Orientation
There are two conflicting definitions in common
usage:
State legislatures, gays, lesbians, mainline and liberal
religious groups, mental health practitioners, human
sexuality researchers and others generally define "sexual
orientation" in terms of the sex of the individual that an
older teen or adult finds sexually, emotionally, and
spiritually attractive:
- A heterosexual is attracted to members of the
opposite sex.
- A homosexual is attracted to members of the same
sex.
- A bisexual is attracted to both men and women.
Sexual orientation thus defines a person's feelings; it
is considered part of their personal makeup.
Some mental health professionals have more precise
definitions which include the sex that an individual focuses
their amorous thoughts, their erotic desires, their sexual
fantasies and their feelings.
Sometimes asexuality is added as a fourth option; this
covers individuals who have no feelings of sexual
attraction. Everyone has a sexual orientation. They may be
sexually active or celibate.
Some religious and social conservatives define "sexual
orientation" much broader in terms of: The sex, and/or
- The age, and/or
- The species, and/or
- The state of the individual that a person finds
sexually, emotionally, and spiritually attractive.
They define sexual orientation as including one or more
of the following terms:
- A heterosexual is attracted to members of the
opposite sex.
- A homosexual is attracted to members of the same
sex.
- A bisexual is attracted to both males and
females.
- A pedophile is attracted to pre-pubescent
children.
- A ephebophile is attracted to young people about the
age of puberty.
- A hebephile is attracted to post-pubertal adolescents
(14 to 17 years of age).
- A necrophile is attracted to dead bodies.
- A zoophile (a.k.a. "zoo") is an individual, almost
always male, who in involved in bestiality or zoophilia:
sexual activities or fantasies involving non-humans.
Etc.
Also included may be individuals who engage in or
fantasize about a variety of paraphilias. "Paraphilia" means
"beyond love" in Greek. The category includes dozen sexual
desires and activities that lie outside the cultural norm
and only involve a minority of adults. Examples are
sadomasochism, exhibitionism, voyeurism, emetophilia,
klismaphilia, etc. See Jahsonic.com
for definitions of these paraphilias. 1
Some quotations from religious an social conservatives
who have apparently taken this broad interpretation of
sexual orientation: Judith Reisman, writing in the
Fundamentalist Christian news service WorldNetDaily in the
year 2000 referred to "...federal 'sexual orientation'
legislation protects all 'orientations' which would include
'affectional' pedophiles, pederasts, sadists, zoophiles,
coprophiles, necrophiles, and the like." 2
In their essay "Sexual Orientation: Fixed or Changeable?,
The Traditional Values Coalition writes: "There are
literally dozens of groups of individuals who engage in
bizarre sexual behaviors and who have mental conditions
known as paraphilias or behaviors known as fetishes. These
include Coprophagia.... Klismaphilia... Pederasty...
Sadomasochism... Diaper fetishes... Necrophilia... All of
these behaviors could be considered to be 'sexual
orientations' -- and many of the individuals who engage in
these behaviors are working to have their peculiar sexual
behaviors declared to be normal in psychiatry and in the
culture at large....Pedophilia and Sadomasochism are just
two of many sexual orientations that may eventually be
normalized in our society." 3
The Fort Worth Star-Telegram reported that: "Cheryl
Surber stood before the City Council and passionately urged
members to kill an effort to have sexual orientation covered
by the citys anti- discrimination ordinances. Such an
ordinance, Surber said, would force Fort Worth employers to
ask all employees, 'Are you a homosexual, heterosexual,
bisexual, asexual, transsexual, transvestite, pedophile,
rapist or celibate'?" 4
Scott Lively of NewsWithViews wrote: " 'Sexual
orientation' is a highly ambiguous term loaded with hidden
false assumptions. An 'orientation' describes the
perspective of a subject toward an object. A sexual
orientation therefore describes a person (subject) by the
object toward which they are sexually attracted: a
homosexual is someone oriented toward someone of the same
sex, a bisexual toward both sexes, a pedophile toward
children, a sado-masochist toward giving or receiving pain,
etc.. By definition, there are an unlimited number of
potential sexual orientations." 5
What do the terms "homosexuality" and "bisexuality"
mean?
Again, there are two conflicting definitions in common
usage:
Gays, lesbians, mainline and liberal religious groups,
mental health practitioners, human sexuality researchers and
others generally define "homosexuality" as feelings of
sexual, emotional, and spiritual attraction to members of
the same sex. A bisexual is a person who is attracted to
both men and women. Everyone, whether heterosexual, bisexual
or homosexual, can decide to be celibate, or be sexually
active. Homosexuality, bisexuality and heterosexuality is
part of what a person is.
Religious and social conservatives generally define
"homosexuality" as a state of being sexually active with
members of the same sex. Homosexuality is what a person
does. The term "bisexuality" is largely ignored.
This lack of agreement on the meanings of words makes
dialogue and debate very difficult.
A person who is sexually attracted to members of the same
sex, but who is not sexually active is considered a
homosexual by the first definition, but not by the
second.
A person who is attracted to both men and women is
considered a bisexual by the first definition. If they
confine their sexual activity to members of the: opposite
sex, they are considered a heterosexual by the second
definition.
same sex, they are considered a homosexual by the second
definition.
A person who is attracted to members of the same sex, was
once sexually active and is now celibate is considered a
homosexual by the first definition but an ex-gay by the
second.
A person who is attracted to both men and women, was once
sexually active with members of the same sex is considered a
bisexual by the first definition, but an ex-gay by the
second.
The confusion continues.
References used to prepare this essay:
"Paraphilia" at: www.jahsonic.com
Judith Reisman, "A platform for pedophiles?,"
WorldNetDaily, 2000-OCT-30, at: worldnetdaily.com
"Sexual Orientation: Fixed or Changeable?, The
Traditional Values Coalition, at: traditionalvalues.org
Kristin N. Sullivan, "Gay Rights Effort May Be Tabled,"
Fort Worth Star-Telegram, 1998-NOV-25, at: www.sodomylaws.org
Scott Lively, "Deciphering 'Gay' Word-Speak and Language
of Confusion," NewsWithViews, 2002-MAY-25, at: www.newswithviews.com
Source: www.religioustolerance.org/hom_prot3.htm
Sexual orientation from Wikipedia,
the free encyclopedia
Sexual orientation, sexual preference or sexual
inclination describes the focus of a person's amorous or
erotic desires, fantasies, and feelings. A person's sexual
orientation is most often classified by the gender(s) one is
"oriented" towards, as:
- heterosexual, if the focus is primarily a person of
the opposite sex,
- homosexual, if the focus is primarily a person of the
same sex;
- bisexual, if it may be a person of either sex
- asexual, if there is no sexual attraction to
anyone
Contents
1 Complexities
and terminology
2 History
of sexuality
3 Orientation
concepts
4 Psychological
and sociological
viewpoints
5 Biological
viewpoint
6 Religious
and moral
viewpoints
7 Sexual
orientation as a
"construction"
8 Sexual
identity
(self-identification)
9 See
also
10 References
11 External
links
Complexities and terminology
Sexual orientation generally refers to how people of
various genders create spontaneous feelings in the
individual, or which orientation a person identifies with
(which may be different). According to some interpretations,
person's sexual behavior and sexual identity
(self-identification) may or may not reflect his/her sexual
orientation. For example, sexual abstinence is independent
of sexual orientation in this sense. Some people who may
self-identify as having a homosexual orientation engage in
heterosexual behavior and even heterosexual marriages to
escape social stigma. Most bisexual people have only one
sexual or romantic partner at a time, and sometimes happen
to have sexual and romantic partners from one only gender
throughout their entire lives, despite attraction to some
people of both sexes. People with heterosexual attractions
may nonetheless have homosexual encounters (including
involving initiation by the other party, multiple
simultaneous partners, acts of deception, or other unusual
social circumstances). A minority of people who
self-identify as heterosexual or homosexual actually feel
attracted to and engage in sexual behavior with people of
both genders.
This situation is complicated further by the fact that
there are several different biological and psychosocial
components to gender, and a given person may not cleanly fit
into a particular category. Some people even find the notion
of distinct genders (and distinct sexual orientations based
upon them) to be offensive. The complexities of gender are
explained in the article on sex.
Sexual fetishism is orthogonal to the gender-based
categories of sexual orientation listed above, though of
course it may in some cases be an important part of a
person's sexual identity and behavior.
Some people feel that various forms of "paraphilia", such
as sexual attraction to animals, (zoophilia), furries, or
inanimate objects are "alternative" sexual orientations to
those listed above. Others argue that these classifications
are orthogonal.
Some people use the term queer as an umbrella term to
include homosexuality and bisexuality, as well as fetishism,
non-human sexual attraction, and other "paraphilia", but it
may also be used more narrowly.
History of sexuality
There have been different views of sexual orientation in
the past. In some cases, a person was considered homosexual,
for example, if and only if they had homosexual sex. This
perspective still defended by some, notably in the works of
the National Association on Research and Therapy for
Homosexuality, a group that breaks with mainstream
pyschological practice in asserting that homosexuality is a
disorder.
In other cases, a person could have homosexual sex on
occasion, but still be considered to be heterosexual. Some
cultures, such as classic Greece and Rome, may have not
classified sexual orientation (if at all) by the gender to
which one is attracted, but by one's social position in
relation to one's position or role during sexual activity.
As heterosexual men in the United States are still expected
to refrain from engaging in sexual activity with other men,
a free Roman male was expected not to be penetrated, with
transgressors being similarly labelled as effeminate.
The term sexual preference was used in the late 20th
century by gay rights advocates promoting the view that each
person should have the right to seek out the partner they
prefer, whether of the opposite sex or the same sex. The
term sexual orientation is now preferred by most gay rights
advocates for its emphasis on fixed sexual identity, as well
as countering the charge by some that their sexuality is a
choice, although both terms still see use.
Orientation concepts
While heterosexuality is considered the statistical or
biological norm, the concept of "normal" and "abnormal" with
its connotations of sickness or moral judgment are no longer
considered valid by mainstream researchers. In 1998, the
American Psychological Association stated that "The reality
is that homosexuality is not an illness. It does not require
treatment and is not changeable." Some conservative
researchers disagree, but their view is not shared by the
most mainstream psychological and medical associations.
Even the belief that heterosexuality is the statistical
norm has been challenged by some researchers, starting with
Alfred C. Kinsey, who claimed that most people's orientation
falls along a gradual scale between the two extremes of
heterosexuality and homosexuality, with society influencing
people to choose socio-normal sexual outlets. Opponents of
Kinsey have claimed that his research methods were not
objective (see Kinsey Reports), notably that Kinsey had
included prison inmates as test subjects.
In a response to these and other criticisms, Paul
Gebhard, Kinsey's successor as director of the Kinsey
Institute for Sex Research, spent years "cleaning" the
Kinsey data of its purported contaminants, removing, for
example, all material derived from prison populations in the
basic sample. In 1979, Gebhard (with Alan B. Johnson)
published The Kinsey Data: Marginal Tabulations of the
1938-1963 Interviews Conducted by the Institute for Sex
Research. Their conclusion, to Gebhard's surprise he
claimed, was that none of Kinsey's original estimates were
significantly affected by this bias.
Some early civilizations, such as those of ancient Greece
and Rome, often accepted homosexual behavior but, in
general, did not make a distinction between homosexuality
and heterosexuality as orientations. Homosexual and
heterosexual responses were considered to both be "natural"
feelings that manifest to a greater or lesser degree in
different individuals. The Greek civilization in particular
considered it quite natural for young men to have older
mentors with whom sexual interaction was accepted. A similar
example was reported in Rome too, with the well known
"Satyricon" by Petronius Arbiter, in which a common
acceptance of pedophilia is also described. There was no
serious inquiry into the causes of sexual orientation,
because there was relatively little awareness of it as a
concept; people were free to follow their personal
inclinations. In a sense, sexual orientation is a social
construct, and a relatively new one. It is most likely
determined by a combination of continually interacting
sociocultural influences and biological proclivities; where
most in most cultures have a sexual object preference for
the opposite sex, much fewer having a sexual object
preference for the same sex, and even fewer having no
preference. There is growing evidence of ambisexuality, or a
"potentiality of bisexuality" where perhaps all people are
capable of attraction to both or either sex, though it is
still likely that people have sexual object preferences.
The traditional Western view that homosexuality was due
to man's rebellious or fallen nature, or demonic temptation
has given way to scientific explanations which regard
homosexuality as natural. Scientists are now questioning the
view that homosexuality is a freely made choice or
"lifestyle" that one has decided to follow, and many
religions are updating their theologies to conform with
science.
Psychological and sociological
viewpoints
For many years the common assumption, shared by many
scientists and religious communities, was that the natural
and normal human sexual orientation is exclusively for the
opposite sex (heterosexual). Sexual studies carried out
during and after the 1950s led psychologists and doctors to
recognize homosexuality as a second exclusive orientation.
Since then similar acceptance has grown for non-exclusive
orientations, such as bisexuality.
Sigmund Freud famously characterized humans as naturally
"polymorphously perverse," meaning either that practically
any object can be a source of erotic fulfillment, or that
babies are relatively indifferent to the object of erotic
fulfillment. Freud argued that as the child grows, the
objects of erotic fulfillment become more clearly defined
and limited (whether this is the result of a biological or a
social process is a matter of debate). Anthropologists have
observed that around the world many people, including people
within the same culture, may be oriented towards a variety
of objects. Nevertheless, most scholars assume that in any
given society what is considered an appropriate object of
desire is highly regulated and limited. Moreover, some
cultural traditions (especially religious) assert that
people should have only one class of objects of desire.
According to two controversial studies, Sexual Behavior
in the Human Male (1948) and Sexual Behavior in the Human
Female (1953) by Dr. Alfred C. Kinsey, when asked to rate
themselves on a continuum from completely heterosexual to
completely homosexual, and when the individuals' behavior as
well as their identify is analyzed, the majority of people
appear to be at least somewhat bisexual, i.e., most people
have some attraction to either sex, although usually one sex
is preferred. According to Kinsey, only a minority (5-10%)
can be considered fully heterosexual or homosexual.
Conversely, only an even smaller minority can be considered
fully bisexual. This led Kinsey to propose what has since
become known as the Kinsey scale. Kinsey concluded that
there are not "two discrete populations, heterosexual and
homosexual.... Only the human mind invents categories and
tries to force facts into pigeonholes. The living world is a
continuum in each and every one of its aspects..."
More recently, attempts to define human sexuality which
factor in concepts of changing sexuality over a persons
lifetime and their internally perceived ideal state, such as
the Klein Sexual Orientation Grid (www.bisexual.org/klein/default.asp#kleingrid
),
have been put forward.
Most modern scientific surveys find that the majority of
people report a mostly heterosexual orientation. However the
relative percentage of the population that reports a
homosexual orientation varies with differing methodologies
and selection criteria. Most of these statistical findings
are in the range of 2.8 to 9 percent of males, and 1 to 5
percent of females for the United States (source:
[1] (www.colorado.edu/Economics/CEA/papers98/wp98-33.pdf
),
page 24 -- this figure can be as high as 12% for some large
cities and as low as 1% percent for rural areas). Almost all
of these studies have found that homosexual males occur
roughly at twice the rate of homosexual females. Estimates
for the percentage of the population that identify as
bisexual vary widely based on the type of questions asked.
Some studies only consider a person bisexual if they are
nearly equally attracted to both sexes, and others consider
a person bisexual if they are at all attracted to the same
sex (for otherwise mostly heterosexual persons) or to the
opposite sex (for otherwise mostly homosexual persons).
(need to find the current estimates and ranges for the
percent of the population that identifies as bisexual)
A very small percentage of people are not sexually
attracted to anyone (asexuality).
Biological viewpoint
Neurological studies had shown that certain regions of
the brain of heterosexuals are dissimilar with homosexuals,
especially when looking at the studies of neuroanatomist
Simon LeVay, a researcher at the Salk Institute. His
research was of the brains of 41 male cadavers (19 of them
homosexual.) He found that the Third Interstitial Nucleus of
the Anterior Hypothalamus (INAH-3) was much smaller in
homosexuals than in the heterosexual men he had also
dissected.
However, LeVay stated as his conclusion: "It's important
to stress what I didn't find. I did not prove that
homosexuality is genetic, or find a genetic cause for being
gay. I didn't show that gay men are born that way, the most
common mistake people make in interpreting my work. Nor did
I locate a gay center in the brain. ...Since I look at adult
brains, we don't know if the differences I found were there
at birth or if they appeared later." [2]
(http://www.fact-index.com/g/ge/genetic_basis_for_homosexuality.html)In
addition to this, of the men LeVay used in his studies, the
sexual histories the "heterosexual" men were unknown. The
man considered the "dean of American sexologists," Johns
Hopkins University psychologist John Money, concerning
LeVay's studies, says, "Of course it [sexual
orientation] is in the brain. The real question is, when
did it get there? Was it prenatal, neonatal, during
childhood, puberty? That we do not know."
Simon LeVay's finding points out a correlation between
physiology and sexual orientation, but does not necessarily
establish -- by itself alone -- a genetic basis for sexual
orientation.
Religious and moral viewpoints
Much religious teaching maintains that sexual behavior
should conform to moral and religious codes. For example,
Christianity has traditionally considered homosexuality to
be morally wrong. Recently, the level of acceptance of
homosexuality within Christianity has, in general,
increased.
Wider issues of sexual morality are also considered by
many religions. Some religions advocate chastity or celibacy
for some members, and many religions condemn incest and
zoophilia. Often religious views of sexual orientation are
based on considerations of what seems natural.
For more see:
Homosexuality and morality
Religion and homosexuality
Sexual morality
Buddhist views of homosexuality
Christian views of homosexuality
Islamic views of homosexuality
Jewish views of homosexuality
Neopagan views of homosexuality
Unification Church views of sexuality
Sexual orientation as a
"construction"
Many people in Western societies today speak of sexual
orientation as a unified and actual thing. Over the past
thirty years some anthropologists, historians, and literary
critics have pointed out that it in fact comprises a variety
of different things, including a specific object of erotic
desire, and forms of erotic fulfillment (i.e. sexual
behaviors). Some scholars in Queer studies have argued that
"sexual orientation" and specific sexual orientations are
historical and social constructions. In 1976 the historian
Michel Foucault argued that homosexuality as a concept did
not exist as such in the 18th century; that people instead
spoke of "sodomy" (which involved specific sexual acts
regardless of the sex of the actors) as a crime that was
often ignored but sometimes punished severely (see sodomy
law). He further argued that it was in the 19th century that
homosexuality came into existence as practitioners of
emerging sciences as well as arts sought to classify and
analyze different forms of sexual perversion. Finally,
Foucault argues that it was this emerging discourse that
allowed some to claim that homosexuality is natural, and
therefore a legitimate sexual orientation.
Foucault's suggestions about Western sexuality led other
historians and anthropologists to abandon the 19th century
project of classifying different forms of sexual behavior or
sexual orientation to a new project that asks "what is
sexuality and how do people in different places and at
different times understand their bodies and desires?" For
example, they have argued that the famous case of some
Melanesian societies in which adult men and pre-pubescent
and adolescent boys engage in oral sex is not comparable to
similar acts in the United States or Europe; that
Melanesians do not understand or explain such acts in terms
of sexual desire or as a sexual behavior, and that it in
fact reflects a culture with a very different notion of sex,
sexuality, and gender. Some historians have made similar
claims about so-called homosexuality in ancient Greece; that
behaviors that appear to be homosexual in modern Western
societies may have been understood by ancient Greeks in
entirely different ways.
At stake in these new views are two different points. One
is the claim that human sexuality is extraordinarily
plastic, and that specific notions about the body and
sexuality are socially constructed. The other is the
fundamentally anthropological claim of cultural relativism:
that human behavior should be interpreted in the context of
its cultural environment, and that the language of one
culture is often inappropriate for describing practices or
beliefs in another culture. A number of contemporary
scholars who have come to reject Foucault's specific
arguments about Western sexuality nevertheless have accepted
these basic theoretical and methodological points.
Critics of the strong social constructionism view that an
underlying phenomenon, sexual orientation (meaning the
tendency for spontaneous erotic desires regarding specific
people of a specific gender or genders) has always existed.
(Usually because it is physiological in origin; this is of
course controversial.) What might be a recent social
invention is the notion of a particular form of sexual
identity (or self-identification) distinction from
orientation.
For more see:
Queer theory
Causes of sexual orientation
Genetics and sexual orientation
Environment, choice, and sexual orientation
Sexual identity
(self-identification)
There are many social, psychological, and political
issues surrounding "identities", "identity groups", or
"communities", which people of various sexual orientations
affiliate themselves.
See also
Affectional orientation
Lesbigay
Queer
Reparative therapy
Sexual behavior
Demographics of sexual orientation
References
Sell, Randall L. (Dec 1997). Defining and measuring
sexual orientation: a review. Archives of Sexual Behavior
26(6) 643-658. (excerpt (www.pbs.org/wgbh/pages/frontline/shows/assault/context/defining.html
)
Gil Brum, Larry McKane, and Gerry Karp. Biology --
Exploring Life, 2nd edition. John Wiley & Sons, Inc.
1994. p. 663. (About INAH-3.)
External links
Wikiquote has a collection of quotations by or about
Sexual orientation.
American Psychological Association:Answers to Your
Questions About Sexual Orientation and Homosexuality
(www.apa.org/pubinfo/answers.html
)
Magnus Hirschfeld Archive of Sexology at the Humboldt
University in Berlin (en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Sexual_orientation
)
How many gay people are there?
It is very difficult to calculate even the approximate
number of gay people, and in estimating even roughly how
many gay people there are anywhere, the following points
have to be kept in mind:
- How many people we estimate are homosexual depends on
how we define homosexuality.
- Many more people experience sexual feelings for
someone of the same sex than report recent sexual
experience with someone of the same sex.
- Because homosexuality is stigmatised it is more
likely to be under than over reported.
How many gay people are there in Britain?
Between 1989-1990, a National Survey of Sexual Attitudes
and Lifestyles (NATSAL) of nearly 19,000 people was
undertaken in Britain. The survey examined a cross section
of people throughout the country, and looked at their sexual
attitudes and behaviour, including people's same sex sexual
experiences. The NATSAL survey was repeated between
1999-2001; this time the subject group involved less people,
at just over 11,000. The results of the study provide us
with an interesting comparison of how same sex sexual
behaviour is changing in Britain.
Among men they found the following results;
NATSAL I (%) 1990 NATSAL II (%) 2000
- Ever had a sexual experience, not necessarily
including genital contact, with a partner of the same
sex? 5.3 8.4
- Ever had sex with a same sex partner, including
genital contact? 3.7 6.3
- Have you had a same sex partner in the last five
years? 1.4 2.6
And among women they found these results;
NATSAL I (%) 1990 NATSAL II (%) 2000
- Ever had a sexual experience, not necessarily
including genital contact, with a partner of the same
sex? 2.8 9.7
- Ever had sex with a same sex partner, including
genital contact? 1.9 5.7
- Have you had a same sex partner in the last five
years? 0.6 2.6
The differences in the two NATSAL surveys clearly
indicate the changes that have been occurring in people's
same sex sexual attitudes and behaviours. The greatest
change is highlighted in the increase in women who have
indicated that they have had a same sex sexual experience,
not necessarily including genital contact, as this increased
from 2.8% in 1990 to 9.7% in 2000. More men said that they
had had a same sex sexual experience too, up from 5.3% in
1990 to 8.4% in 2000, thus showing that either same sex
sexual behaviour is either on the increase or that people
are more willing to report it.
When looking at same sex sexual attraction, and not
necessarily sexual experience, the figures have also changed
over the ten-year period, with women showing the most
significant difference. In 1990, 93.3% of men said they had
only ever had sexual attraction towards the opposite sex,
whilst by 2000 this had fallen to 91.9%. 93.6% of women in
1990 said they had only ever been attracted to men, but by
2000 this had dropped to 88.3%. From this we can therefore
deduce that 11.7% of women and 8.1% of men have felt a
sexual attraction towards the same sex at least once in
their lives. 1, 2
How many young gay people are there in
Britain?
Among men they found the following results;
NATSAL I (%) 1990 NATSAL II (%) 2000 16-17 yrs 18-19 yrs
20-24 yrs NATSAL II Average, 16-24 years
- Ever had a sexual experience with a same sex partner?
4.3 1.2 5.6 6.3 4.3
- Ever had sexual intercourse/genital contact with a
same sex partner? 2.4 1.2 2.3 4.2 2.6
And among women they found these results;
NATSAL I (%) 1990 NATSAL II (%) 2000 16-17 yrs 18-19 yrs
20-24 yrs NATSAL II Average, 16-24 years
- Ever had a sexual experience with a same sex partner?
3.0 5.1 9.7 12.6 9.1
- Ever had sexual intercourse/genital contact with a
same sex partner? 1.4 2.5 4.6 6.5 4.5
By adding the three figures for each category in NATSAL
II, and getting an average percentage, we can at least gain
some comparison between the two sets of data. The
differences are again most apparent in women, especially in
the sexual experience category, with the figure rising from
only 3.0% in 1990 to 9.1% in 2000. The differences in men
are less obvious, with the average for sexual experience for
2000 being the same as 1990, at 4.3%. What you can deduce
from the male data is that same sex sexual experience does
increase more rapidly over the age of 18 years; the data for
18-19 years and 20-24 years, at 5.6% and 6.3% respectively,
is higher than the whole 16-24 year average of 4.3%.
How many gay people are there in America?
As well as the problems outlined above in estimating the
number of gay people, the geographical size and dense
population presents an additional problem for anybody trying
to calculate the number of gay people in America.
However, Alfred Kinsey carried out research in the 1940s
and 1950s into the sexual behaviour of 12,000 men and women
in America. Although his work has been heavily criticised it
remains a rare and relatively authoritative study of sexual
behaviour in America.
Amongst the men he found:
- 37% reported some homosexual contact;
- 13% reported more homosexual than heterosexual
contact;
- 4% reported exclusively homosexual contact.
Amongst the women he found:
- 13% reported some homosexual contact;
- 4% reported more homosexual than heterosexual
contact;
- 1% reported exclusively homosexual contact.
From these results Kinsey realized that not only were few
people exclusively homosexual, but also far from the vast
majority, particularly of men, were exclusively
heterosexual. This led him to develop an orientation scale,
which had exclusively heterosexual and exclusively
homosexual at opposite ends with a wide middle range to
cater for the majority of people who were neither.
More recently, data has been collected in America, during
the ten-year national census, on married and
unmarried-partner households. They did not ask the actual
sexual orientation of the respondents, so there is no
measure of single gay people, nor is their a measure of
those gay people in committed relationships but not living
together. Whilst the census cannot give us a figure for the
number of people who are gay in America, it can inform us on
how many same-sex partnership households identified
themselves in the survey. The 2000 census tells us that
- there are 105.5 million households in the USA.
- 5.5 million of these consist of unmarried
partnerships,
- of these, 595,000 consist of same sex partners.
This can be interpreted as there being nearly 1.2 million
gay people living with a same sex partner in America. This
is a huge increase from the 1990 census, which identified
only 145,000 same sex unmarried households. As with the
NATSAL survey in the UK, there is undoubtedly a large amount
of under reporting in these sorts of surveys. Possible
explanations of this include continued prejudice and
discrimination against gay people.3
There have been various other surveys in the US that have
tried to measure numbers of gay people. An analysis of these
surveys by the Human Rights Campaign came up with this
conclusion.
'In the last three elections, the Voter News Service exit
poll registered the gay vote between 4 percent and 5
percent. While concluding that the Census 2000 undercounted
the total number of gay or lesbian households, for the
purposes of this study, we estimate the gay and lesbian
population at 5 percent of the total U.S. population over 18
years of age, (209,128,094). This results in an estimated
total gay and lesbian population of 10,456,405. A recent
study of gay and lesbian voting habits conducted by Harris
Interactive determined that 30 percent of gay and lesbian
people are living in a committed relationship in the same
residence. Using that figure, we suggest that 3,136,921 gay
or lesbian people are living in the United States in
committed relationships in the same residence. '4
So, if we accept that the data presented by the Human
Rights Commission is indeed indicative of the real numbers,
then it shows that the census data is only showing up a
small percentage of the actual number of gay people living
in America. Until, however, a nationwide survey is done,
asking questions on sexual attitudes and behaviour, then we
can only use data and analyses, such as the ones above, to
work out an answer to the question of gay people living in
America.
Is homosexuality more common now?
Throughout history there have always been people who have
had homosexual feelings and experiences. In fact, in some
cultures, at some times, these feelings have been celebrated
or at least accepted rather than stigmatised. However two
major changes in most of Europe and America have taken
place, which may have given the impression that
homosexuality is now more prevalent than at any other
time.
Images of gay and lesbian people, their lifestyles and
the issues they face have become very much more visible in
recent years. There are gay and lesbian characters and
story-lines in soap operas (e.g. East-Enders (UK),
Coronation Street (UK), Melrose Place (USA)), situation
comedies (Will and Grace (USA), Absolutely Fabulous (UK)),
and very powerful gay and lesbian images and image-makers in
popular music and culture (e.g. Pet Shop Boys, Will Young,
R.E.M, K.D.Lang,). Programs where contestants rely on public
telephone votes to remain or to win have been won by out Gay
and Lesbian people for example "Big Brother (UK)", "Fame
Academy" and "Popstars (UK)".
At the same time there have been a series of important
struggles for gay and lesbian equality. In Britain this has
been marked by demands for the right to be parents (adoptive
and natural), the right to fair treatment at work and so on.
These have, as has often been intended, attracted
considerable media, political and legal attention.
Text adapted from "Talking about Homosexuality in the
Secondary School" book. The book can be downloaded for free
from Printable resources
Resources
1 'National Survey of Sexual Attitudes and Lifestyles I',
Kaye Wellings, Julia Field, Anne M. Johnson, et al, Penguin,
1994, 0140158146
2 'National Survey of Sexual Attitudes and Lifestyles II:
Reference Tables and Summary Report', Bob Erens, Sally
McManus, Alison Prescott, et al, National Centre for Social
Research, 2003, 1904599028
3 'Married and Unmarried-Partner Households by
Metropolitan Residence Status: 2000', United States Census
2000, US Census Bureau, http://www.census.gov/
4 'Gay and Lesbian Families in the United States:
Same-Sex Unmarried Partner Households. A Preliminary
Analysis of 2000 United States Census Data'. August 22,
2001, David M. Smith, and Gary J. Gates, Human Rights
Campaign, www.hrc.org
Source:
www.avert.org/hsexu1.htm
Attempts To Change Sexual
Orientation "Reparative therapy."
The mass media and the Web are filled with claims these days
from religious conservatives, orthodox psychoanalysts,
anti-gay organizations, and even a professional football
player claiming that people with a homosexual orientation
not only can become heterosexual, but also should do so.
However, claims by the Family Research Council, Charles
Socarides, Joseph Nicolosi, and others of "successful"
conversions through reparative therapy are filled with
methodological ambiguities and questionable results (for
reviews, see Haldeman, 1991, 1994; see also Haldeman's 1999
review paper is available on the web in HTML and Adobe
Acrobat (PDF) format). They are also ethically suspect.
In many of these behavior-change techniques, "success"
has been defined as suppression of homoerotic response or
mere display of physiological ability to engage in
heterosexual intercourse. Neither outcome is the same as
adopting the complex set of attractions and feelings that
constitute sexual orientation.
Many interventions aimed at changing sexual orientation
have succeeded only in reducing or eliminating homosexual
behavior rather than in creating or increasing heterosexual
attractions. They have, in effect, deprived individuals of
their capacity for sexual response to others. These
"therapies" have often exposed their victims to electric
shocks or nausea-producing drugs while showing them pictures
of same-sex nudes (such techniques appear to be less common
today than in the past).
Another problem in many published reports of "successful"
conversion therapies is that the participants' initial
sexual orientation was never adequately assessed. Many
bisexuals have been mislabeled as homosexuals with the
consequence that the "successes" reported for the
conversions actually have occurred among bisexuals who were
highly motivated to adopt a heterosexual behavior
pattern.
The extent to which people have actually changed their
behavior even within the confines of these inadequate
operational definitions often has not been
systematically assessed. Instead, only self reports of
patients or therapists' subjective impressions have been
available. More rigorous objective assessments (e.g.,
behavioral indicators over an extended period of time) have
been lacking (Coleman, 1982; Haldeman, 1991, 1994; Martin,
1984).1
Some psychoanalysts claim to have conducted empirical
research demonstrating that their "therapies" are able to
change gay people into heterosexuals. Their studies have
multiple flaws, including a lack of safeguards against bias
and a lack of control groups. Rather than having patients
evaluated by an independent third party who is unaware of
which patients received the "reparative therapy," these
studies are simply compilations of self-reports from
psychoanalysts who are attempting to change their patients'
sexual orientation (and who are highly motivated to report
"success").
And even if we accept these studies' claim that change
has occurred, they do not provide any evidence that such
change resulted from a particular therapy. Individuals who
changed might well have done so anyway, even without
therapy.
The 2001 APA Convention Papers
Furor Erupts Over Study On Sexual Orientation, an article
by Ken Hausman in the APA's Psychiatric News (July 6,
2001)
Claims about the "success" of conversion therapies have
appeared mainly in the mass media and on the World Wide Web,
rather than in high-quality, peer-reviewed scientific
journals. A 2000 paper by Joseph Nicolosi and his colleagues
was published in Psychological Reports. Psychological
Reports is also the major outlet for Paul Cameron, the
discredited antigay psychologist. As detailed elsewhere on
this site, Psychological Reports has very low prestige among
researchers and a low rejection rate. In addition, unlike
other psychological journals, it charges its authors a fee
to publish their papers.
However, in May of 2001, two papers on the topic of
conversion therapies were presented at the American
Psychiatric Association's annual convention. One paper, by
Dr. Robert Spitzer, reported findings from 45-minute
telephone interviews with 143 men and 57 women who had
sought help to change their sexual orientation. He found
that 66 percent of the men and 44 percent of the women had
achieved "good heterosexual functioning" and he attributed
this to the interventions.
The Spitzer study was immediately criticized on several
grounds. For example, the sample consisted predominantly of
activists recruited from "ex-gay" and anti-gay
organizations. About two thirds were referred to Spitzer by
so-called "ex-gay ministries," such as Exodus, or by the
National Association for Research and Therapy of
Homosexuality (NARTH). Of those who participated, 78 percent
had spoken publicly in favor of efforts to convert
homosexuals to heterosexuality.
This is a potential weakness of the study because
activists are highly motivated to report that they
successfully changed their sexual orientation. Consequently,
they may present an inaccurate impression of themselves to
researchers. Dr. Spitzer took the activists' testimonials at
face value, with no checks on the reliability or validity of
their self-reports. In his relatively brief interviews with
them, Dr. Spitzer may not have been able to detect factual
errors or misstatements intentional or inadvertent
by the activists.
Dr. Spitzer's study also appears to suffer from some of
the same methodological flaws as the published studies
discussed above. For example, only a minority of the
participants (about 40%) were exclusively attracted to
partners of the same sex before they attempted to change. As
noted above, including bisexuals in studies evaluating the
outcomes of conversion therapies tends to inflate the
proportion of "successes."
Dr. Spitzer did not claim that his findings could be
generalized to the gay and lesbian population at large.
Indeed, he was quoted in the New York Times as saying that,
despite the findings from his study, the number of
homosexuals who could successfully become heterosexual was
likely to be "pretty low." He also conceded that
participants in his study were "unusually religious" and
were not necessarily representative of most gay men and
lesbians in the United States.
The second APA paper, presented by Dr. Ariel Shidlo and
Dr. Michael Schroeder, reported findings from a study of 202
homosexuals who were recruited through the Internet and
direct mailings to groups advocating conversion therapy.
Most of the participants (178, or 88%) reported that efforts
to change their sexual orientation had failed. Only 6 (3%)
achieved what the researchers considered a heterosexual
shift. Drs. Shidlo and Schroeder also reported that many
respondents were harmed by the attempt to change.
The research by Drs. Shidlo and Schroeder has recently
been published (Schroeder & Shidlo, 2001; Shidlo &
Schroeder, 2002). Dr. Spitzer's research is currently being
reviewed for publication.
An Analogy To better appreciate the potential flaws in
Dr. Spitzer's study, consider an analogous situation.
Suppose a pharmaceutical company claims that a new
vitamin supplement can change left-handed people to
right-handers. Mainstream medical organizations express
their opposition to the vitamin, saying that it causes harm
to many people who use it, and noting that there is no
reason for left-handed people to try to change.
To test the drug company's claim, a researcher conducts
brief telephone interviews with people who have used the
product. He recruits most of his research participants from
a list (provided by the drug company) of individuals who
claim to have used the vitamin and have given public
testimonials on behalf of the drug company. Many of those
people say that they tried the vitamin because they felt
miserable as left-handers in a right-handed world, and that
they are now functioning as right-handers (although many
report occasional thoughts about using their left hand).
The researcher's data consist entirely of the one-time
telephone interviews. He does no follow-up interviews to
assess the consistency of the users' stories. Nor does he
conduct face-to-face assessments with standardized measures
to assess whether the vitamin users have actually become
right-handed. Meanwhile, another research team reports data
from a different study, in which they found that the vitamin
supplement did not change most left-handers to
right-handers, and that many people who tried the vitamin
suffered serious negative side effects.
In such a situation, we would want to ask several
questions. How reliable are the reports of vitamin users who
were recruited through the drug company? What about the many
people who were harmed by the vitamin? Why is it important
for left-handers to become right-handed in the first
place?
We can raise similar questions about Dr. Spitzer's
study.
How reliable are the reports of people recruited through
Exodus and NARTH?
For those who did change, how do we know that they would
not have changed their sexual orientation anyway, even
without some form of therapy?
What about the many people who have been harmed by
conversion therapies?
Why is it important for gay men and lesbians to become
heterosexual in the first place? Doesn't the real problem
lie in society's hostility toward people who are homosexual
or bisexual?
Indeed, even if conversion therapies could be shown to
change sexual orientation in a small number of cases, there
are strong arguments that doing so is unethical. For
example, Dr. Gerald Davison (1991), a former president of
the Association for the Advancement of Behavior Therapy,
argued that change-of-orientation programs are ethically
improper, and that their availability only confirms
professional and societal biases against homosexuality.
The Mainstream Position
For more than a quarter-century, the major professional
associations of mental health practitioners and researchers
in the United States have recognized that homosexuality is
not a mental illness. They are highly critical of attempts
to change sexual orientation.
The American Psychiatric Association's official web site
notes that: "There is no published scientific evidence
supporting the efficacy of 'reparative therapy' as a
treatment to change one's sexual orientation.... There are a
few reports in the literature of efforts to use
psychotherapeutic and counseling techniques to treat persons
troubled by their homosexuality who desire to become
heterosexual; however, results have not been conclusive, nor
have they been replicated. There is no evidence that any
treatment can change a homosexual person's deep seated
sexual feelings for others of the same sex. Clinical
experience suggests that any person who seeks conversion
therapy may be doing so because of social bias that has
resulted in internalized homophobia, and that gay men and
lesbians who have accepted their sexual orientation
positively are better adjusted than those who have not done
so."
Text of the 1997 resolution
The American Psychiatric Association's Position
Statements on conversion therapy are posted on their web
site.
At its meeting in August, 1997, the Council of
Representatives of the American Psychological Association
overwhelmingly approved a resolution affirming its longtime
position that homosexuality is not a disorder and raising
serious questions about so-called reparative therapies. In
particular, the APA resolution raised the question of
whether it is ethically possible for a psychologist to
conduct conversion therapy with individuals who are not
capable of informed consent, including minors.
In 1998, at its December 11-12 meeting, the American
Psychiatric Association Board of Trustees unanimously
endorsed a position statement opposing reparative therapy.
According to the 1998 position statement:
"The potential risks of 'reparative therapy' are great,
including depression, anxiety and self-destructive behavior,
since therapist alignment with societal prejudices against
homosexuality may reinforce self-hatred already experienced
by the patient."
"Many patients who have undergone 'reparative therapy'
relate that they were inaccurately told that homosexuals are
lonely, unhappy individuals who never achieve acceptance or
satisfaction."
"The possibility that the person might achieve happiness
and satisfying interpersonal relationships as a gay man or
lesbian is not presented, nor are alternative approaches to
dealing with the effects of societal stigmatization
discussed."
"Therefore, the American Psychiatric Association opposes
any psychiatric treatment, such as 'reparative' or
'conversion' therapy which is based upon the assumption that
homosexuality per se is a mental disorder or based upon a
prior assumption that the patient should change his/her
homosexual orientation. The American Psychiatric Association
recognizes that in the course of ongoing psychiatric
treatment, there may be appropriate clinical indications for
attempting to change sexual behaviors."
In Summary
In summary, scientific data are lacking to show that
behavior modification techniques effectively change
individuals' sexual orientations from homosexual to
heterosexual. The relatively small number of attempts that
have been adequately documented appear to have been largely
unsuccessful.
Does this mean that no one ever changes his or her
orientation from homosexual to heterosexual through the use
of such techniques? Not necessarily. It is possible that
some individuals who enter such therapies eventually make
such a change, although there is no evidence for a
cause-and-effect relationship. Those people might have
changed their sexual orientation without the therapy.
However, so-called reparative therapy techniques
premised on the assumption that homosexuality is a form of
psychopathology appear to do much more harm than
good. And even if conversion therapies were shown to be
successful in more than a relative handful of cases, they
would remain ethically questionable.
The mainstream view in psychology and psychiatry is that
people who are troubled about their homosexual orientation
have internalized society's prejudice against homosexuality,
and that the appropriate task of a therapist is to help them
to overcome those prejudices and to lead a happy and
satisfying life as a gay man or lesbian.
Notes
1. Claims by religious organizations to have changed
homosexuals to heterosexuals generally have not been
documented in such a way as to permit their critical
evaluation. For more discussion of them, see Haldeman (1991,
1994). (return to text)
"Ex-Gay Leader Confronted In Gay Bar" from the Southern
Voice (September 23, 2000).
John Paulk, "a prominent ex-gay leader once featured as
'going straight' on the cover of Newsweek magazine, was
confronted and photographed" patronizing a gay bar in
Washington, D.C. According to the Exodus web site (currently
inactive), he subsequently was removed as Chairman of the
Board of Exodus North America.
Calculated Compassion: How the Ex-Gay Movement Serves the
Right's Attack on Democracy (Political Research
Associates)
Source: psychology.ucdavis.edu/rainbow/html/facts_changing.html
Bibliography for Facts About Sexual
Orientation
Note: This bibliography accompanies the web pages for
"Facts About Sexual Orientation." It is not intended to
serve as a general resource guide for research on sexual
orientation.
American Psychiatric Association. (1987). Diagnostic and
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (3rd ed., Revised).
Washington, DC: Author.
American Psychological Association. (1974). Standards for
educational and psychological tests. Washington, DC:
Author.
American Psychological Association. (1975). Minutes of
the Council of Representatives. American Psychologist, 30,
633.
American Psychological Association. (1987). Policy
Statements on Lesbian and Gay Issues. Washington, DC:
Author.
Bayer, R. (1987). Homosexuality and American psychiatry:
The politics of diagnosis (2nd Ed.). Princeton, NJ:
Princeton University Press.
Bérubé, A. (1990). Coming out under fire:
The history of gay men and women in World War II. New York:
Free Press
Bieber, I., Dain, H., Dince, P., Drellich, M., Grand, H.,
Gundlach, R., Kremer, M., Rifkin, A., Wilbur, C., &
Bieber, T. (1962). Homosexuality: A psychoanalytic study of
male homosexuals. New York: Basic Books.
Boswell, J. (1980). Christianity, social tolerance and
homosexuality. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Brown, R.D., & Cole, J.K. (1985). Letter to the
Editor. Nebraska Medical Journal, 70, 410-414.
Bryant, A. (1977). The Anita Bryant story: The survival
of our nation's families and the threat of militant
homosexuality. Old Tappan, NJ: Fleming H. Revell.
Cameron, P. (1985). Homosexual molestation of
children/sexual interaction of teacher and pupil.
Psychological Reports, 57, 1227-1236.
Cameron, P., Proctor, K., Coburn, W., Forde, N., Larson,
H., & Cameron, K. (1986). Child molestation and
homosexuality. Psychological Reports, 58, 327-337.
Chauncey, G., Jr. (1982/1983). From sexual inversion to
homosexuality: Medicine and the changing conceptualization
of female deviance. Salmagundi, No. 58-59, 114-146.
Cochran, S. D., Keenan, C., Schober, C., & Mays, V.
M. (2000). Estimates of alcohol use and clinical treatment
needs among homosexually active men and women in the U.S.
population. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
68(6), 1062-1071.
Cochran, S. D., & Mays, V. M. (2000). Relation
between psychiatric syndromes and behaviorally defined
sexual orientation in a sample of the US population.
American Journal of Epidemiology, 151(5), 516-23.
Cochran, S. D., & Mays, V. M. (2000). Lifetime
prevalence of suicide symptoms and affective disorders among
men reporting same-sex sexual partners: Results from NHANES
III. American Journal of Public Health, 90, 573-578.
Cochran, W.G., Mosteller, F., & Tukey, J.W. (1954).
Statistical problems of the Kinsey report. Washington, DC:
American Statistical Association.
Coleman, E. (1982) Changing approaches to the treatment
of homosexuality: A review. In W. Paul, J. Weinrich, J.
Gonsiorek & M. E. Hotvedt (Eds.), Homosexuality: Social,
Psychological, and Biological Issues (pp. 81-88). Thousand
Oaks: Sage.
Davison, G.C. (1991). Constructionism and morality in
therapy for homosexuality. In J. Gonsiorek & J. Weinrich
(Eds.), Homosexuality: Research implications for public
policy (pp. 137-148). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
D'Emilio, J., & Freedman, E.B. (1988). Intimate
matters: A history of sexuality in America. New York: Harper
& Row.
Duberman, M.B., Vicinus, M., & Chauncey, G., Jr.
(1989). Hidden from history: Reclaiming the gay and lesbian
past. New York: New American Library.
Ellis, H. (1901). Studies in the psychology of sex:
Volume 2: Sexual inversion. Philadelphia: F.A. Davis.
Erickson, W.D., Walbek, N.H., & Seely, R.K. (1988).
Behavior patterns of child molesters. Archives of Sexual
Behavior, 17 (1), 77-86.
Fay, R.E., Turner, C.F., Klassen, A.D., & Gagnon,
J.H. (1989). Prevalence and patterns of same-gender sexual
contact among men. Science, 243, 338-348.
Fenichel, O. (1945). The psychoanalytic theory of
neurosis. New York: W.W. Norton.
Finkelhor, D. (1984). Child sexual abuse: New theory and
research. New York: Free Press.
Finkelhor, D., & Araji, S. (1986). Explanations of
pedophilia: A four factor model. The Journal of Sex
Research, 22 (2), 145-161.
Ford, C.S., & Beach, F.A. (1951). Patterns of sexual
behavior. New York: Harper & Brothers.
Freedman, M. (1971). Homosexuality and psychological
functioning. Belmont, CA: Brooks/Cole.
Freud, S. (1905). Three essays on the theory of
sexuality. In J. Strachey (Ed. and Trans.), The standard
edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund
Freud. (Vol. 7, pp. 123-245). London: Hogarth Press.
(Original work published 1905)
Freund, K., Watson, R., & Rienzo, D. (1989).
Heterosexuality, homosexuality, and erotic age preference.
The Journal of Sex Research, 26 (1), 107-117.
Gilman, S. E., Cochran, S. D., Mays, V. M., Hughes, M.,
Ostrow, D., & Kessler, R. C. (2001). Risk of psychiatric
disorders among individuals reporting same-sex sexual
partners in the National Comorbidity Survey. American
Journal of Public Health, 91(6), 933-939.
Gonsiorek, J.C. (1982). Results of psychological testing
on homosexual populations. American Behavioral Scientist, 25
(4), 385-396.
Gonsiorek, J.C. (1991). The empirical basis for the
demise of the illness model of homosexuality. In J.
Gonsiorek & J. Weinrich (Eds.), Homosexuality: Research
implications for public policy (pp. 115-136). Thousand Oaks,
CA: Sage.
Groth, A.N., & Birnbaum, H.J. (1978). Adult sexual
orientation and attraction to underage persons. Archives of
Sexual Behavior, 7 (3), 175-181.
Groth, A. N., & Gary, T. S. (1982). Heterosexuality,
homosexuality, and pedophilia: Sexual offenses against
children and adult sexual orientation. In A.M. Scacco (Ed.),
Male rape: A casebook of sexual aggressions (pp. 143-152).
New York: AMS Press.
Groth, A.N., Hobson, W.F., & Gary, T.S. (1982). The
child molester: Clinical observations. Journal of Social
Work and Human Sexuality, 1 (1/2), 129-144.
Haldeman, D.C. (1991). Conversion therapy for gay men and
lesbians: A scientific examination. In J. Gonsiorek & J.
Weinrich (Eds.), Homosexuality: Research implications for
public policy (pp. 149-160). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Haldeman, D.C. (1994). The practice and ethics of sexual
orientation conversion therapy. Journal of Consulting and
Clinical Psychology, 62 (2), 221-227.
Hart, M., Roback, H., Tittler, B., Weitz, L., Walston,
B., & McKee, E. (1978). Psychological adjustment of
nonpatient homosexuals: Critical review of the research
literature. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 39(7),
604-608.
Hatfield, L. (1989, June 5). Method of polling. San
Francisco Examiner, p.A-20.
Herdt, G.H. (Ed.) (1984). Ritualized homosexuality in
Melanesia. Berkeley: University of California Press.
Hooker, E. (1957). The adjustment of the male overt
homosexual. Journal of Projective Techniques, 21, 18-31.
Jenny, C., Roesler, T. A., & Poyer, K. L. (1994). Are
children at risk for sexual abuse by homosexuals?
Pediatrics, 94(1), 41-44.
Johnson, R.L., & Shrier, D. (1987). Past sexual
victimization by females of male patients in an adolescent
medicine clinic population. American Journal of Psychiatry,
144, 650-652.
Jones, E. (1957). Sigmund Freud: Life and work (Vol. 3).
London: Hogarth.
Katz, J. N. (1976). Gay American history: Lesbians and
gay men in the USA. New York: Thomas Y. Crowell Company.
Kinsey, A.C., Pomeroy, W.B., & Martin, C.E. (1948).
Sexual behavior in the human male. Philadelphia: W.B.
Saunders.
Kinsey, A.C., Pomeroy, W.B., Martin, C.E., & Gebhard,
P.H. (1953). Sexual behavior in the human female.
Philadelphia: W.B. Saunders.
Klassen, A. D., Williams, C. J., & Levitt, E. E.
(1989). Sex and morality in the U.S.: An empirical enquiry
under the auspices of the Kinsey Institute. Middletown, CT:
Wesleyan University Press.
Knight, R. A. (1989). An assessment of the concurrent
validity of a child molester typology. Journal of
Interpersonal Violence, 4(2), 131-150.
Lauman, E.O., Gagnon, J.H., Michael, R.T., &
Michaels, S. (1994). The social organization of sexuality:
Sexual practices in the United States. Chicago: University
of Chicago Press.
Lever, J., & Kanouse, D.E. (1996). Sexual orientation
and proscribed sexual behaviors. In G. Herek, J. Jobe, &
R. Carney (Eds.), Out in force: Sexual orientation and the
military (pp. 15-38). Chicago: University of Chicago
Press.
Lewes, K. (1988). The psychoanalytic theory of male
homosexuality. New York: Simon and Schuster.
Martin, A.D. (1984). The emperor's new clothes: Modern
attempts to change sexual orientation. In T. Stein & E.
Hetrick (Eds.), Innovations in psychotherapy with
homosexuals (pp. 24-57). Washington, DC: American
Psychiatric Press.
Mays, V. M., & Cochran, S. D. (2001). Mental health
correlates of perceived discrimination among lesbian, gay,
and bisexual adults in the United States. American Journal
of Public Health, 91(11), 1869-76.
McConaghy, N. (1998). Paedophilia: A review of the
evidence. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry,
32(2), 252-265.
Newton, D. E. (1978). Homosexual behavior and child
molestation: A review of the evidence. Adolescence, 13,
29-43.
Rado, S. (1940). A critical examination of the concept of
bisexuality. Psychosomatic Medicine, 2, 459-467.
Rado, S. (1949). An adaptational view of sexual behavior.
In P.H. Hoch & J. Zubin (Eds.), Psychosexual development
in health and disease (pp. 159-189). New York: Grune and
Stratton.
Reiss, B.F. (1980). Psychological tests in homosexuality.
In J.Marmor (Ed.), Homosexual behavior: A modern reappraisal
(pp. 296-311). New York: Basic Books.
Results of poll. (1989, June 6). San Francisco Examiner,
p. A-19.
Robinson, P. (1976). The modernization of sex. New York:
Harper & Row.
Rogers, S.M., & Turner, C.F. (1991). Male-male sexual
contact in the USA: Findings from five sample surveys,
1970-1990. The Journal of Sex Research, 28, 491-519.
Sandfort, T. G. M., de Graaf, R., Bijl, R. V., &
Schnabel, P. (2001). Same-sex sexual behavior and
psychiatric disorders: Findings from the Netherlands mental
health survey and incidence study (NEMESIS). Archives of
General Psychiatry, 58(1), 85-91.
Schroeder, M., & Shidlo, A. (2001). Ethical issues in
sexual orientation conversion therapies: An empirical study
of consumers. Journal of Gay and Lesbian Psychotherapy,
5(3-4), 131-166.
Shidlo, A., & Schroeder, M. (2002). Changing sexual
orientation: A consumers' report. Professional Psychology:
Research and Practice, 33(3), 249-259.
Silverstein, C. (1991). Psychological and medical
treatments of homosexuality. In J. Gonsiorek & J.
Weinrich (Eds.), Homosexuality: Research implications for
public policy (pp. 101-114). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.
Socarides, C. (1968). The overt homosexual. New York:
Grune and Stratton.
Terman, L.M. (1948). Kinsey's "Sexual Behavior in the
Human Male": Some comments and criticisms. Psychological
Bulletin, 45, 443-459.
Voeller, B. (1990). Some uses and abuses of the Kinsey
scale. In D.P. McWhirter, S.A. Sanders, & J.M. Reinisch
(Eds.), Homosexuality/heterosexuality: Concepts of sexual
orientation (pp. 32-38). New York: Oxford University
Press.
Wallis, W.A. (1949). Statistics of the Kinsey report.
Journal of the American Statistical Association, 44,
463-484.
Williams, W.L. (1986). The spirit and the flesh: Sexual
diversity in American Indian culture. Boston: Beacon.
Source: psychology.ucdavis.edu/rainbow/html/facts_bibliography.html
Source: www.barackobama.com/dadt-repealed?source=20110920_jm_nd&utm_medium=email&utm_source=obama&utm_campaign=20110920_jm_nd&email=gordonclay@aol.com&zip=97123&firstname=Gordon&lastname=Clay&keycode=319757243ea5062be9edc3421207a1a9fbacf908fb15925137591ce14168633a
* * *
"I'd rather have a gay guy who can play than a straight
guy who can't play." Charles Barkley, Former NBA player
Contact
Us |
Disclaimer
| Privacy
Statement
Menstuff®
Directory
Menstuff® is a registered trademark of Gordon Clay
©1996-2023, Gordon Clay
|